Because it is laid out in columns, this site doesn't display properly on a phone held vertically.
Please turn it sideways.
‘There is no point in using exact methods where there is no clarity in the concepts and issues to which they are to be applied.’1
This page is not yet published. Go back
‘There is no point in using exact methods where there is no clarity in the concepts and issues to which they are to be applied.’1
In the introduction to his book The technology of text, David Jonassen2 described his topic as
a counterpoint to the artistic and unsystematic approach to text design and layout that has prevailed since petroglyphs were first inscribed on walls.
I beg to differ. We could equally say that the best text design and layout is a counterpoint to the ignorant and often atheoretical experiments that some scientists have done with very poorly designed materials.
Research on information design needs careful handling. Typographic variables interact with each other, so the larger the typeface, for example, the fewer words on a line of equal length. As long ago as 1931, BR Buckingham3 criticized the univariate research model, in which experimenters try to vary a single factor while holding all others constant. He comments:
This is a good experimental technique. It is an article of faith among investigators. Yet it won’t work in the way it has been applied to typography unless one is prepared to go to very unusual lengths with it.’ (p. 104)
Happily for information designers, it seems that there is strong agreement between published research and the cumulative practice of printers and designers since the invention of printing. Gordon Legge (a psychologist) and Charles Bigelow (a typographer) compared type sizes in common use, and sizes recommended by research. As they put it:
We present evidence supporting the hypothesis that the distribution of print sizes in historical and contemporary publications falls within the psychophysically defined range of fluent print size, the range over which text can be read at maximum speed.4
No one should embark on scientific research on typography without reading Ole Lund’s PhD thesis on the sorry history of this endeavour.5 He quotes this remark by RB Joynson, a psychologist:
it is altogether too easy to assume that, when the psychologist’s conclusions run counter to common sense, it is the psychologist’s conclusions which are correct and common sense which was wrong; and it is altogether too easy to assume that, when the psychologist’s conclusions agree with common sense, what was previously only guessed has now been reliably established.6
When reading scientific research on information design, my tips are:
Karel van der Waarde and Myra Thiessen have written a detailed set of nineteen questions to ask as you read research reports.5
1. John von Neumann and Oskar Morgenstern (1944) Theory of Games and Economic Behavior, Princeton University Press.
2. Jonassen D (ed) (1982) The technology of text, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technology Publications
3. Buckingham BR (1931) ‘New data on the typography of textbooks’ , Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, 30, 93–125. He goes on to point out that to do a full study of even a modest range of typefaces, sizes, line lengths and line spacings would require more effort than anyone is prepared to put in (he outlines a simple study that he claims would have required 1,792,000 responses).
4. Legge, G. E., & Bigelow, C. A. (2011). Does print size matter for reading? A review of findings from vision science and typography. Journal of Vision, 11(5):8, 1–22, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/5/8, doi:10.1167/11.5.8.
5. Lund, Ole. 1999. Knowledge construction in typography: The case of legibility research and the legibility of sans serif typefaces. PhD thesis, University of Reading.
6. Joynson, R.B. 1974. Psychology and common sense. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
7. van der Waarde, K., & Thiessen, M. (forthcoming 2025). Nineteen questions to evaluate typographic research: Chaff and wheat. Visible Language, 59(1).